VR Stereoscope

A simple smartphone-based stereoscope

Header Image



Usage


  1. Acquire some smartphone VR glasses. We suggest something cheap and foldable that does not obstruct your touchscreen, like the Homido Mini or Pocket 360.
  2. Launch a browser on your phone. In Android use Chrome; in iOS use Safari.
  3. Point your phone's browser to the current page, either by typing the URL or by scanning this QR code:
  4. In your phone's browser, click one of the stereo thumbnails in the galleries below
  5. Enter VR Mode by hitting the VR button and rotating your phone to landscape orientation
  6. To return to this page from VR Mode, touch your phone's screen to reveal the browser's title bar and then click the Back button


Troubleshooting:

  • If the stimulus appears blurry or strange in VR mode, make sure your VR glasses are perfectly centered and aligned on your phone. It may take some fiddling.
  • If Safari's title bar does not disappear in VR Mode, try this:
    1. Close all browser tabs except for the current page
    2. Rotate your phone to portrait orientation and then back to landscape
    3. If all else fails, you can at least reduce its size by clicking aA -> Hide Toolbar in the address bar

Gallery 2: Interactive Perceptual Stimuli (Beta)


These are interactive versions, where the stimulus is meant to change as you rotate your head horizontally.


Interactive Unpaired Bar
(Nakayama and Shimojo, 1990)

Interactive Cross
(Nakayama et al, 1990)

Interactive Folded Paper
(Von Szily, 1921, Ehrenstein and Gillam, 1998)

Gallery 3: Photographs

Some examples from the Middlebury 2014 Stereo Dataset

Pipes
Jadeplant


Gallery 4: Use Your Own


View your own images stereoscopically by pasting two URLs and clicking Go.

(Higher resolution images produce a better effect but take longer to load. If your screen is persistently blank in VR mode, check the image server's access controls.)




Another way to see your own images is by passing their URLs as query strings. The syntax is
(base URL)?lImg=(left image URL)&rImg=(right image URL)

For example, an equivalent way to launch the Two Planes stimulus above is by typing
StereoPages/static.html?lImg=/stereoscope/assets/StereoImages/1_1/2048one_l.png
&rImg=/stereoscope/assets/StereoImages/1_1/2048one_r.png


References for Perceptual Stimuli


  • A. von Szily. Stereoskopische Versuche mit Schattenrissen. Albrecht von Graefes Archiv fur Ophthalmologic, 105 964-972 und Tafel VI, 1921.
  • B. Julesz. Binocular depth perception without familiarity cues. Science, 145(3630):356–362, 1964.
  • K. Nakayama and S. Shimojo. Da Vinci Stereopsis: Depth and subjective occluding contours from unpaired image points. Vision research, 30(11):1811–1825, 1990.
  • K. Nakayama and S. Shimojo. Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces. Science, 257(5075):1357-63, 1992.
  • B.L. Anderson and K. Nakayama. Toward a general theory of stereopsis: binocular matching, occluding contours, and fusion. Psychological review, 101(3):414, 1994.
  • P.N. Belhumeur. A Bayesian approach to binocular steropsis. International Journal of Computer Vision, 19(3):237–260, 1996.
  • W.H. Ehrenstein and B.J. Gillam. Early demonstrations of subjective contours, amodal completion, and depth from half-occlusions: “Stereoscopic experiments with silhouettes” by Adolf von Szily (1921). Perception, 27(12):1407-16, 1998.
  • I. Tsirlin, L.M. Wilcox, and R.S. Allison. Monocular occlusions determine the perceived shape and depth of occluding surfaces. Journal of Vision, 10(6):11–11, 2010.